Pharmacopsychiatry 2019; 52(03): 148-154
DOI: 10.1055/a-0590-4850
Original Paper
© Georg Thieme Verlag KG Stuttgart · New York

Peripheral Oxytocin Predicts Higher-Level Social Cognition in Men Regardless of Empathy Quotient

Lisa Deuse
1   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Psychiatry, Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
,
Olga Wudarczyk
1   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Psychiatry, Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
,
Lena Rademacher
2   Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Social Neuroscience Lab, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
,
Peter Kaleta
1   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Psychiatry, Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
,
Wolfram Karges
3   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Clinic for Gastroenterology, Metabolic Disorders and Internal Intensive Medicine, Aachen, Germany
,
Stella Kacheva
3   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Clinic for Gastroenterology, Metabolic Disorders and Internal Intensive Medicine, Aachen, Germany
,
Gerhard Gründer
4   Department of Molecular Neuroimaging, Central Institute of Mental Health, Clinical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
,
Sarah E. Lammertz
1   Universitatsklinikum RWTH Aachen, Psychiatry, Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Aachen, Germany
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

received 05 February 2018
revised 28 February 2018

accepted 05 March 2018

Publication Date:
28 March 2018 (online)

Abstract

Introduction Pharmaceutical oxytocin (OT) administration is being tested as a novel treatment for social deficits in various psychiatric populations. However, little is known about how naturally occurring variation in peripheral OT relates to differences in social cognition. This study investigates whether healthy individuals with very high or very low levels of empathy differ in endogenous OT and whether OT plasma levels can predict performance in a mentalizing task.

Methods 40 healthy men were included based upon their score above the 85th or below the 15th percentile of the empathy quotient inventory [1]. Participants’ abilities to interpret social information was assessed via the Social Detection Task [2]. Plasma OT levels were analyzed using enzyme immunoassay.

Results OT plasma levels predicted mentalizing performance for more ambiguous social scenes (i. e., difficult items) for all participants. We found no group differences in OT plasma levels between subjects with high and low empathy.

Discussion These findings confirm a link between peripheral OT and the ability to read subtle nonverbal social cues in healthy individuals, which is independent of self-reported empathy.

 
  • References

  • 1 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord 2004; 34: 163-175
  • 2 Deuse L, Rademacher LM, Winkler L. et al. Neural correlates of naturalistic social cognition: brain-behavior relationships in healthy adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016; 11: 1741-1751
  • 3 Anacker AM, Beery AK. Life in groups: the roles of oxytocin in mammalian sociality. Front Behav Neurosci 2013; 7: 185
  • 4 Neumann ID. Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males. J. Neuroendocrinol 2008; 20: 858-865
  • 5 Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 2009; 30: 548-557
  • 6 Hoertnagl CM, Hofer A. Social cognition in serious mental illness. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2014; 27: 197-202
  • 7 Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD. et al. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 2011; 35: 573-588
  • 8 Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Eack SM. et al. Correlates of social functioning in autism spectrum disorder: the role of social cognition. Res Autism Spectr Disord 2017; 35: 25-34
  • 9 Eckstein M, Hurlemann R. Oxytozin. Der Nervenarzt 2013; 84: 1321-1328
  • 10 Keri S, Benedek G. Oxytocin enhances the perception of biological motion in humans. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2009; 9: 237-241
  • 11 Schulze L, Lischke A, Greif J. et al. Oxytocin increases recognition of masked emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011; 36: 1378-1382
  • 12 Domes G, Kumbier E, Heinrichs M. et al. Oxytocin promotes facial emotion recognition and amygdala reactivity in adults with Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014; 39: 698-706
  • 13 Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A. et al. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61: 731-733
  • 14 Bernaerts S, Berra E, Wenderoth N. et al. Influence of oxytocin on emotion recognition from body language: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 72: 182-189
  • 15 Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry 2016; 79: 194-202
  • 16 Gamer M, Zurowski B, Buchel C. Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107: 9400-9405
  • 17 Groppe SE, Gossen A, Rademacher L. et al. Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 74: 172-179
  • 18 Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N. et al. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15: 301-309
  • 19 Olff M, Frijling JL, Kubzansky LD. et al. The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38: 1883-1894
  • 20 Gossen A, Hahn A, Westphal L. et al. Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration – a study in healthy men. Neuropeptides 2012; 46: 211-215
  • 21 Rubin LH, Carter CS, Bishop JR. et al. Reduced levels of vasopressin and reduced behavioral modulation of oxytocin in psychotic disorders. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40: 1374-1384
  • 22 Feldman R, Weller A, Zagoory-Sharon O. et al. Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol Sci 2007; 18: 965-970
  • 23 Feldman R, Gordon I, Schneiderman I. et al. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 1133-1141
  • 24 Engert V, Koester AM, Riepenhausen A. et al. Boosting recovery rather than buffering reactivity: higher stress-induced oxytocin secretion is associated with increased cortisol reactivity and faster vagal recovery after acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 74: 111-120
  • 25 Gouin JP, Carter CS, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H. et al. Marital behavior, oxytocin, vasopressin, and wound healing. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35: 1082-1090
  • 26 Nishizato M, Fujisawa TX, Kosaka H. et al. Developmental changes in social attention and oxytocin levels in infants and children. Sci. Rep 2017; 7: 2540
  • 27 Rubin LH, Carter CS, Drogos L. et al. Peripheral oxytocin is associated with reduced symptom severity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010; 124: 13-21
  • 28 Strauss GP, Keller WR, Koenig JI. et al. Endogenous oxytocin levels are associated with the perception of emotion in dynamic body expressions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2015; 162: 52-56
  • 29 Strauss GP, Keller WR, Koenig JI. et al. Plasma oxytocin levels predict social cue recognition in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2015; 162: 47-51
  • 30 Lancaster K, Carter CS, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H. et al. Plasma oxytocin explains individual differences in neural substrates of social perception. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9: 132
  • 31 Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N. et al. Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol Sci 2010; 21: 1426-1428
  • 32 Feeser M, Fan Y, Weigand A. et al. Oxytocin improves mentalizing – pronounced effects for individuals with attenuated ability to empathize. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015; 53: 223-232
  • 33 Hecht EE, Robins DL, Gautam P. et al. Intranasal oxytocin reduces social perception in women: neural activation and individual variation. NeuroImage 2017; 147: 314-329
  • 34 Strang S, Gerhardt H, Marsh N. et al. A matter of distance – the effect of oxytocin on social discounting is empathy-dependent. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017; 78: 229-232
  • 35 Parker KJ, Oztan O, Libove RA. et al. Intranasal oxytocin treatment for social deficits and biomarkers of response in children with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017; 114: 8119-8124
  • 36 Miller M, Bales KL, Taylor SL. et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: sex differences and associations with symptoms. Autism Res 2013; 6: 91-102
  • 37 Modahl C, Green LA, Fein D. et al. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43: 270-277
  • 38 Green L, Fein D, Modahl C. et al. Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alterations in peptide forms. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50: 609-613
  • 39 Alabdali A, Al-Ayadhi L, El-Ansary A. Association of social and cognitive impairment and biomarkers in autism spectrum disorders. J Neuroinflammation 2014; 11: 4
  • 40 Husarova VM, Lakatosova S, Pivovarciova A. et al. Plasma oxytocin in children with autism and its correlations with behavioral parameters in children and parents. Psychiatry Investig 2016; 13: 174-183
  • 41 Xu XJ, Shou XJ, Li J. et al. Mothers of autistic children: lower plasma levels of oxytocin and Arg-vasopressin and a higher level of testosterone. PloS One 2013; 8: e74849
  • 42 Zaki J, Ochsner K. The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009; 1167: 16-30
  • 43 Preckel K, Kanske P, Singer T. On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2018; 19: 1-6
  • 44 Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Germine LT. et al. Mentalizing about emotion and its relationship to empathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2008; 3: 204-217
  • 45 Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Germine LT. et al. Neural activity during social signal perception correlates with self-reported empathy. Brain Res 2010; 1308: 100-113
  • 46 Wagner DD, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF. Individual differences in the spontaneous recruitment of brain regions supporting mental state understanding when viewing natural social scenes. Cereb Cortex 2011; 21: 2788-2796
  • 47 de Vries GJ. Sex differences in vasopressin and oxytocin innervation of the brain. Prog Brain Res 2008; 170: 17-27
  • 48 Gossen A, Groppe SE, Winkler L. et al. Neural evidence for an association between social proficiency and sensitivity to social reward. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9: 661-670
  • 49 Lawrence EJ, Shaw P, Baker D. et al. Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychol Med 2004; 34: 911-919
  • 50 Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Uchiyama T. et al. Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions: cross-cultural stability. J Autism Dev Disord 2007; 37: 1823-1832
  • 51 Wright DB, Skagerberg EM. Measuring empathizing and systemizing with a large US sample. PloS One 2012; 7: e31661
  • 52 Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM. et al. The temperament and character inventory (TCI). A guide to Its Development and Use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1994
  • 53 Lubben J. Assessing social networks among elderly populations. Fam Community Health 1988; 11: 42-52
  • 54 Kanning UP. Inventar sozialer kompetenzen. Goettingen: Hogrefe Verlag; 2009
  • 55 Motoki K, Sugiura M, Takeuchi H. et al. Are plasma oxytocin and vasopressin levels reflective of amygdala activation during the processing of negative emotions? A preliminary study. Front Psychol 2016; 7: 480
  • 56 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J. et al. The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42: 241-251
  • 57 Goldman M, Marlow-O’Connor M, Torres I. et al. Diminished plasma oxytocin in schizophrenic patients with neuroendocrine dysfunction and emotional deficits. J. Schizophr Res 2008; 98: 247-255
  • 58 Rubin LH, Carter CS, Drogos L. et al. Sex-specific associations between peripheral oxytocin and emotion perception in schizophrenia. J. Schizophr Res 2011; 130: 266-270
  • 59 Keri S, Kiss I, Kelemen O. Sharing secrets: oxytocin and trust in schizophrenia. Soc Neurosci 2009; 4: 287-293
  • 60 Parker KJ, Garner JP, Libove RA. et al. Plasma oxytocin concentrations and OXTR polymorphisms predict social impairments in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014; 111: 12258-12263
  • 61 Yuen KW, Garner JP, Carson DS. et al. Plasma oxytocin concentrations are lower in depressed vs. healthy control women and are independent of cortisol. J Psychiatr Res 2014; 51: 30-36
  • 62 Bertsch K, Schmidinger I, Neumann ID. et al. Reduced plasma oxytocin levels in female patients with borderline personality disorder. Horm Behav 2013; 63: 424-429
  • 63 LoParo D, Waldman ID. The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 20: 640-646
  • 64 Montag C, Sindermann C, Melchers M. et al. A functional polymorphism of the OXTR gene is associated with autistic traits in caucasian and asian populations. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2017; 174: 808-816
  • 65 Melchers M, Montag C, Markett S. et al. Relationship between oxytocin receptor genotype and recognition of facial emotion. Behav Neurosci 2013; 127: 780-787
  • 66 Rault JL. Effects of positive and negative human contacts and intranasal oxytocin on cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 69: 60-66
  • 67 Rutigliano G, Rocchetti M, Paloyelis Y. et al. Peripheral oxytocin and vasopressin: Biomarkers of psychiatric disorders? A comprehensive systematic review and preliminary meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 2016; 241: 207-220
  • 68 Dumais KM, Veenema AH. Vasopressin and oxytocin receptor systems in the brain: sex differences and sex-specific regulation of social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 2016; 40: 1-23
  • 69 Leng G, Sabatier N. Measuring oxytocin and vasopressin: bioassays, immunoassays and random numbers. J Neuroendocrinol 2016; 28: 10.1111/jne.12413
  • 70 Carson DS, Berquist SW, Trujillo TH. et al. Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma oxytocin concentrations are positively correlated and negatively predict anxiety in children. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20: 1085-1090
  • 71 Landgraf R, Neumann ID. Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Front Neuroendocrinol 2004; 25: 150-176