CC BY-NC-ND 4.0 · Sports Med Int Open 2022; 6(01): E1-E8
DOI: 10.1055/a-1720-6083
Physiology & Biochemistry

Influence of Wearing Blue Lenses on Melatonin Production and Performance in Volleyball Players

Eduardo Baptista
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
Rhai André Arriel
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
Ana Luiza de Castro Carvalho
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
Matheus M. C. Bispo
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
Alex Batista Rodrigues
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
Hiago Souza
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
,
2   Department of Sport Sciences/Institute of Health Sciences, Federal University of Triangulo Mineiro, UBERABA, Brazil
,
Moacir Marocolo
1   Institute of Biological Sciences, Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brazil
› Author Affiliations

Abstract

We analyzed the effects of wearing blue lenses on melatonin level, physical and cognitive performance. Fifteen youth volleyball players (15.0±1.5 yrs) attended the laboratory on 3 occasions (48-h interval): on the 1st visit they were familiarized with the procedures of the study, and on 2nd and 3rd visits they were submitted to the testing protocol wearing transparent (control) or blue lens glasses in a counterbalanced crossover design. The protocol consisted of 10 min in “total darkness,” 30 min of light stimulation (wearing blue or transparent lenses), followed by an attentional test, and an agility T-test (without wearing the glasses). Samples of saliva (to determine melatonin concentration) were obtained pre- and post-exposure (30 min) to artificial light, wearing the lenses. Sleepiness, alertness, attention, mood, and perceived recovery status and performance variables (reaction time and T-test) were assessed after lens exposure. Melatonin levels did not differ within and between groups (blue lenses, pre: 0.79±0.73 and post: 1.19±1.374 pg/dl, p=0.252, effect size (ES)=0.38; control, pre: 0.97±1.00 and post: 0.67±0.71 pg/dl, p=0.305, ES=–0.35). Nonetheless, melatonin differences were significantly correlated with physical sedation for glasses with blue lenses (r=−0.526; p=0.04). No other variables differed (p>0.05) between protocols, including T-test performance (p=0.07; ES=0.41). Blue lenses do not influence melatonin levels, cognitive/physical performance, and mood status in amateur youth volleyball players.



Publication History

Received: 16 August 2021
Received: 03 November 2021

Accepted: 05 December 2021

Article published online:
21 February 2022

© 2022. The Author(s). This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonDerivative-NonCommercial-License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit. Contents may not be used for commercial purposes, or adapted, remixed, transformed or built upon. (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Georg Thieme Verlag KG
Rüdigerstraße 14, 70469 Stuttgart, Germany

 
  • References

  • 1 van Bommel W, van den Beld G. Lighting for work: a review of visual and biological effects. Light Res Technol 2004; 36: 255-266
  • 2 Hardeland R, Cardinali DP, Srinivasan V. et al. Melatonin – a pleiotropic, orchestrating regulator molecule. Prog Neurobiol 2011; 93: 350-384
  • 3 Pandi-Perumal SR, Trakht I, Srinivasan V. et al. Physiological effects of melatonin: role of melatonin receptors and signal transduction pathways. Prog Neurobiol 2008; 85: 335-353
  • 4 Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK. et al. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science 1980; 210: 1267-1269
  • 5 Beaven CM, Ekström J. A comparison of blue light and caffeine effects on cognitive function and alertness in humans. PLoS One 2013; 8: e76707
  • 6 Cajochen C, Münch M, Kobialka S. et al. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate to short wavelength light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2005; 90: 1311-1316
  • 7 Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Blattner P. et al. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert?. PLoS One 2011; 6: e16429
  • 8 Papamichael C, Skene DJ, Revell VL. Human nonvisual responses to simultaneous presentation of blue and red monochromatic light. J Biol Rhythms 2012; 27: 70-78
  • 9 Viola AU, James LM, Schlangen LJM. et al. Blue-enriched white light in the workplace improves self-reported alertness, performance and sleep quality. Scand J Work Environ Health 2008; 34: 297-306
  • 10 Voss MW, Kramer AF, Basak C. et al. Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl Cogn Psychol 2010; 24: 812-826
  • 11 Sheppard JM, Gabbett TJ, Stanganelli L-CR. An analysis of playing positions in elite men’s volleyball: considerations for competition demands and physiologic characteristics. J Strength Cond Res 2009; 23: 1858-1866
  • 12 Posner MI. Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 1980; 32: 3-25
  • 13 Knufinke M, Nieuwenhuys A, Maase K. et al. Effects of natural between-days variation in sleep on elite athletes’ psychomotor vigilance and sport-specific measures of performance. J Sports Sci Med 2018; 17: 515-524
  • 14 Brainard GC, Hanifin JP, Greeson JM. et al. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J Neurosci 2001; 21: 6405-6412
  • 15 Thapan K, Arendt J, Skene DJ. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J Physiol 2001; 535: 261-267
  • 16 Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH. et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28: 193-213
  • 17 Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976; 4: 97-110
  • 18 Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 1990; 52: 29-37
  • 19 Sahin L, Figueiro MG. Alerting effects of short-wavelength (blue) and long-wavelength (red) lights in the afternoon. Physiol Behav 2013; 116–117: 1-7
  • 20 Zuardi AW, Karniol IG. Estudo transcultural de uma escala de auto-avaliacao para estados subjetivos. J bras psiquiatr 1981; 30: 403-406
  • 21 Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T. et al. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 2002; 14: 340-347
  • 22 Benedito-Silva AA, Menna-Barreto L, Marques N. et al. A self-assessment questionnaire for the determination of morningness-eveningness types in Brazil. Prog Clin Biol Res 1990; 341B: 89-98
  • 23 Bertolazi AN, Fagondes SC, Hoff LS. et al. Validation of the Brazilian Portuguese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Sleep Med 2011; 12: 70-75
  • 24 Benloucif S, Burgess HJ, Klerman EB. et al. Measuring melatonin in humans. J Clin Sleep Med 2008; 04: 66-69
  • 25 Rahman SA, Hilaire MA, Gronfier C. et al. Functional decoupling of melatonin suppression and circadian phase resetting in humans. J Physiol 2018; 596: 2147-2157
  • 26 Norris H. The action of sedatives on brain stem oculomotor systems in man. Neuropharmacology 1971; 10: 181-191
  • 27 Sassi RH, Dardouri W, Yahmed MH. et al. Relative and absolute reliability of a modified agility T-test and its relationship with vertical jump and straight sprint. J Strength Cond Res 2009; 23: 1644-1651
  • 28 Laurent CM, Green JM, Bishop PA. et al. A practical approach to monitoring recovery: development of a perceived recovery status scale. J Strength Cond Res 2011; 25: 620-628
  • 29 Hopkins WG, Marshall SW, Batterham AM. et al. Progressive statistics for studies in sports medicine and exercise science. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2009; 41: 3-13
  • 30 Londe AM, Marocolo M, Marocolo IC. et al. Wearing colored glasses can influence exercise performance and testosterone concentration?. Sports Med Int Open 2018; 2: E46-E51
  • 31 Appleman K, Figueiro MG, Rea MS. Controlling light-dark exposure patterns rather than sleep schedules determines circadian phase. Sleep Med 2013; 14: 456-461
  • 32 Higuchi S, Fukuda T, Kozaki T. et al. Effectiveness of a red-visor cap for preventing light-induced melatonin suppression during simulated night work. J Physiol Anthropol 2011; 30: 251-258
  • 33 Lockley SW, Evans EE, Scheer FAJL. et al. Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 2006; 29: 161-168
  • 34 Dorrian J, Lamond N, Holmes AL. et al. The ability to self-monitor performance during a week of simulated night shifts. Sleep 2003; 26: 871-877
  • 35 Wright HR, Lack LC, Kennaway DJ. Differential effects of light wavelength in phase advancing the melatonin rhythm. J Pineal Res 2004; 36: 140-144
  • 36 Zhou X, Ferguson SA, Matthews RW. et al. Mismatch between subjective alertness and objective performance under sleep restriction is greatest during the biological night. J Sleep Res 2012; 21: 40-49
  • 37 Askaripoor T, Motamedzadeh M, Golmohammadi R. et al. Non-image forming effects of light on brainwaves, autonomic nervous activity, fatigue, and performance. J Circadian Rhythms 2018; 16: 9
  • 38 Chang A-M, FAJL Scheer, Czeisler CA. et al. Direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans depend on prior light history. Sleep 2013; 36: 1239-1246
  • 39 Figueiro MG, Sahin L, Wood B. et al. Light at night and measures of alertness and performance: implications for shift workers. Biol Res Nurs 2016; 18: 90-100
  • 40 Fontani G, Maffei D, Cameli S. et al. Reactivity and event-related potentials during attentional tests in athletes. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 1999; 80: 308-317
  • 41 Cajochen C, Zeitzer JM, Czeisler CA. et al. Dose-response relationship for light intensity and ocular and electroencephalographic correlates of human alertness. Behav Brain Res 2000; 115: 75-83
  • 42 Souman JL, Tinga AM, Te Pas SF. et al. Acute alerting effects of light: a systematic literature review. Behav Brain Res 2018; 337: 228-239
  • 43 Leichtfried V, Mair-Raggautz M, Schaeffer V. et al. Intense illumination in the morning hours improved mood and alertness but not mental performance. Appl Ergon 2015; 46 Pt A: 54-59
  • 44 Plitnick B, Figueiro M, Wood B. et al. The effects of red and blue light on alertness and mood at night. Ligh Res Technol 2010; 42: 449-458
  • 45 Hanifin JP, Lockley SW, Cecil K. et al. Randomized trial of polychromatic blue-enriched light for circadian phase shifting, melatonin suppression, and alerting responses. Physiol Behav 2019; 198: 57-66