Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the reader will be able to (1) identify the pragmatic
language skills that children who are deaf or hard of hearing have the most difficulty
mastering by 7 years of age; (2) identify the pragmatic language skills that are the
latest developed with complex language for children with normal hearing; (3) differentiate
the rate of pragmatic language development by age and degree of hearing loss; (4)
compare and contrast the pragmatic language skills of children with normal hearing
and children who are deaf or hard of hearing by age; and (5) develop teaching strategies
for preschool-aged children who are deaf or hard of hearing to develop specific pragmatic
language skills that are typically developed significantly later than children with
normal hearing.
Early identified children who are deaf or hard of hearing (DHH) who are enrolled in
a timely manner into appropriate early intervention programs with providers who have
specialized skills and knowledge in developmentally appropriate, parent-centered intervention
with specialization in deafness and hearing loss have developed language skills in
some areas that are similar to their typically hearing peers. Yoshinaga-Itano et al[1] found that children who are DHH with normal cognition who are identified and receive
intervention prior to 6-months of age, develop language skills at 7 years of age within
the normal range of normally hearing peers on tests of expressive and receptive language—the
Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test and the Test for Auditory Comprehension
of Language. Unlike the generation prior to universal newborn hearing screening with
appropriate early intervention services, this population is able to maintain a rate
of development established in the early years of life. As a result, children who previously
gained language at a rate of 45 to 65% of the rate of normally hearing children, now
have the capability of gaining language at 80 to 100% the rate of children with normal
hearing. The focus on the development of language skills for children who are DHH
has concentrated on vocabulary development and size of the lexicon as well as receptive
and expressive syntax. Although these are essential parts of language development
that facilitate communication, one important aspect of language development in this
population has been largely overlooked. Having the tools of vocabulary, expressive,
and receptive language are indeed essential for communication. However, it is also
important that children are able to use these tools effectively in social interactions
with peers. This important component of language development that addresses an individual's
ability to use language skills appropriately in social context is called pragmatics. It has been an aspect of language development at the most abstract level.
Several studies have assessed the pragmatic development of children with disorders
that affect a child's ability to communicate with peers, such as autism or specific
language impairment (SLI). However, deafness and hearing loss are disorders that greatly
impact communication, yet have not been widely studied in the realm of pragmatic development.
Thus, it is essential that researchers understand the pragmatic skills of DHH children
to determine whether there is a need for pragmatic skills intervention to support
communication.
One study in particular, which looked at 11-year-old children with SLI, found a significant
correlation between pragmatic language difficulties and poor social outcome as well
as expressive language related to victimization.[2] These researchers assessed a group of 242 11-year-old children using a variety of
questionnaires, which were administered to the children as well as their teachers.
Pragmatic difficulties were determined using the Children's Communication Checklist,
which is a commonly used questionnaire to determine pragmatic language impairment
separate from typical SLI. Victimization was based on scores from the My Life in School
questionnaire, which is filled out by the child and can predict whether she or he
is at risk for victimization. Using regression analysis, the most prominent correlation
was found between responses to these two instruments.
Similar studies have been done to assess “social cognition” of children with SLI.
Research conducted by Marion Farmer[3] assessed social cognition of four different groups of 10- and 11-year-old children:
(1) children with SLI attending a special school, (2) children with SLI attending
a mainstream school, (3) control group with chronological age-matched peers without
SLI, and (4) control group with language age-matched peers without SLI. The results
indicated that children with SLI in the special school showed significant deficits
in social competence in comparison to the other three groups. These findings provide
insight into the social language use of children with communication deficits, such
as SLI, who are not integrated into a mainstream school. These results suggest that
children who are at risk for communication difficulties, such as children with hearing
loss, should receive intervention in the mainstream classroom to prevent lower social
competence than children in specialized schools.
Additional studies have found that children between the ages of 7 and 9 years of age
who score high on the Children's Communication Checklist, thus identifying pragmatic
language impairment, are identified by teachers as having socioemotional difficulties.[4] Thus, children with pragmatic difficulties are likely to be perceived as having
deficits in social and emotional development. Children who are DHH may also have socioemotional
deficits that are manifested in pragmatic difficulties due to reduced communication
abilities impacted by difficulties in access to both spoken and visual language. The
question still remains, however, of how DHH children who were identified early (prior
to 6 months, with the majority by 3 months of age) develop pragmatically as compared
with normally hearing peers.
Though research specifically done on pragmatic development of children who are DHH
is not prevalent, several studies have looked at multiple aspects of language development
of DHH children, including language use. The recurring theme found in this literature
indicates that children who are DHH who communicate using oral language are found
to use more directive (instrumental) and less informational (heuristic) language skills
than expected for their chronological age when compared with normally hearing peers.[5]
[6]
[7] Nicholas[6] for example, assessed the use of directing, instrumental, and heuristic language
in children with profound hearing loss as compared with normally hearing children.
Results from this study determined that 3-year-old children with hearing loss who
have mature language skills (e.g., increasing vocabulary size and sentence length)
display social interactions using appropriate informational language skills. This
suggests that DHH children who develop age-appropriate language milestones are likely
to develop age-appropriate informative pragmatic skills as well.
Furthermore, researchers have found that 3-year-old children who are DHH who communicate
orally, are more likely to use more directive communication functions than informative
functions as compared with their normally hearing age-matched peers.[7] These findings suggest that the pragmatic development of DHH children is delayed
when compared with the typical pragmatic development of normally hearing peers. However,
these studies, conducted in the 1990s and early 2000s, included predominantly children
who were not identified through universal newborn hearing screening programs.
Most et al[8] conducted a study of 24 children with hearing loss, 13 children with hearing aids
and 11 children with cochlear implants, between the ages of 6.3 and 9.4 years of age
compared with 13 children with normal hearing. Pragmatic abilities were assessed with
the pragmatic protocol of Prutting and Kirchner.[9] Children with hearing aids and those with cochlear implants had similar pragmatic
language abilities. Children with hearing loss had significantly less effective and
less flexible pragmatic language abilities than their normally hearing peers.
Preliminary data from Yoshinaga-Itano[10] described the pragmatic skills of 54 DHH preschool age children (3- and 4-year-olds).
Pragmatic skills were determined using the Pragmatics Checklist,[11]
[12] which looks at seven categories: instrumental (“I want”), regulatory (“Do as I tell
you”), interactional (“me and you”), personal (“Here I come”), heuristic (“Tell me
why”), imaginative (“Let's pretend”), and informative (“I've got something to tell
you”). These categories have previously been used to describe pragmatic skills of
typically hearing preschool-age children.[12] Of the mentioned categories, skills were rated as either “not present” or “regularly
present.” In this group of children with hearing loss, 3-year-old pragmatic skills
that were most often (≥50% of responses) “not present” included: giving directions,
revision of unclear messages, respect for alternative points of view, explanation
of feelings, telling an adult what is not understood in accusation, offering opinions
on an issue and supplying supportive statements for opinion, supplying basic identification
and biographic data, asking questions for clarification, creating stories with beginning-logical
events-conclusion, using precise noun/pronoun referents, engaging in evaluation of
an object in contrast to another, and evaluating the quality of an event. Skills rated
most often (≥50% of responses) as “not present” in the 4-year-old group included:
revision of unclear message and evaluating the quality of an event. These data are
difficult to interpret because there is no data on normal hearing age-matched peers
using this Pragmatics Checklist instrument. Thus, it was essential that the Pragmatics
Checklist be distributed to typically hearing preschool-aged children so comparisons
can be made between pragmatic development of DHH preschoolers to typically hearing
preschoolers.
In the 1999 analysis, the Pragmatic Checklist outcomes for children who are DHH had
significant and strong relationships to the Minnesota Comprehension-Conceptual (a
measure of receptive language), the Minnesota Expressive Language, and the MacArthur
Expressive Vocabulary subtests at both the 3- and 4-year-old levels. At the 3-year
age level, the Pragmatic Checklist subtests were not significantly related to the
MacArthur Expressive Vocabulary subtest. The pragmatic language skills were significantly
related to the Minnesota Child Development Inventory (MCDI) subtests and ranged from
r = 0.45 to 0.84. At the 4-year age level, the correlations ranged from r = 0.69 to 0.85 for the Minnesota subtests and r = 0.55 to 0.84 for the MacArthur subtest. The seven pragmatic categories at the 4-year
age level were highly related to the Personal-Social subtest of the Minnesota, r = 0.49 to 0.81, but at the 3-year level only three of the seven categories had a
significant relationship to the Personal-Social subtest: instrumental (r = 0.45), heuristic (r = 0.59), imaginative (r = 0.58). This finding may indicate that the interaction between social interaction
and pragmatic language skills becomes stronger as the child ages, as does the relationship
between vocabulary and pragmatic language skills.
If a reciprocal relationship between pragmatic language skills, vocabulary growth,
and syntax development exists, improvement in any aspect should be beneficial to the
other aspects of language. Although the typically developing child with normal hearing
learns pragmatic language skills through incidental learning, the child with a significant
hearing loss often requires specific instruction to understand the importance of these
skills and to learn the content knowledge.
Methodology
Participants with Normal Hearing
The subject population included children ages 2- to 7-years-old with normal hearing.
This project focused on collecting data for ∼30 to 50 children in each age group.
Seven demographic questions existed on this questionnaire to ensure that the control
group met inclusion/exclusion criteria and to mitigate confounding variables that
may affect interpretation of the obtained results. The seven demographic questions
asked for the following: child's birth date and gender, parental level of education,
child's diagnoses, child's educational setting (daycare or preschool), child's ethnicity,
and languages spoken in the home. To match the control group (normal hearing children)
with the experimental group (DHH children), the demographic questions asked on the
large-scale study were also asked on an online questionnaire. Child's age, gender,
ethnicity, language, and educational setting are important for matching the control
and experimental groups as closely as possible. The mother's level of education was
necessary because research shows that maternal level of education is a predictive
factor in DHH language outcomes.[13] A diagnosis of additional disabilities including autism, speech/language disorder,
or cognitive/developmental delay was also requested because these diagnoses are known
to impact communication skills.
Inclusion criteria were: a signed consent form, questionnaire completed in its entirety,
child within the age criteria of 2- to 7-years-old and whose cognitive age is equivalent
to his or her chronological age. Exclusion criteria were: no consent form or missing
information on the questionnaire, children who do not meet the age criteria, children
diagnosed with developmental delays or with a cognitive age below their chronological
age, children with diagnosis of autism or speech/language disorder, or children diagnosed
with hearing loss. Children with developmental or cognitive delays were not to be
included in this study because research emphasizes that DHH children with age-appropriate cognition who receive intervention by 6 months of age are able to develop age-appropriate language
skills similar to typically hearing children.[14] Children with autism, speech/language disorder, or other additional disabilities
impacting language development were excluded from this study. The main goal for this
control group of normally hearing children is that they have typically developing
communication and language skills.
Instrument for Participants
The Pragmatic Checklist with 45 items was adapted from Simon[12] (see [Appendix]). There were 96 total items on the online survey, one question asking for consent
to participate, seven demographic questions, 45 pragmatic checklist items, and 43
situation comprehension survey items. The Pragmatics Checklist is a questionnaire
for parents to describe their child's pragmatic skills. It includes six categories:
states needs, gives commands, personal, interactional, wants explanations, and shares
knowledge and imaginations. Each category consists of several pragmatic skills for
parents to rate their child's ability as not present, uses no words (preverbal), uses
one to three words, and uses more complex language. For example, under the objective
of shares knowledge and imaginations, parents rate the child's ability to role-play
with different characters, role-play with props, retell a story that has been told
to them, etc. A copy of the checklist is located in the [Appendix].
APPENDIX
Child's Name: _______________________________________
|
Person Completing Form: ______________________________
|
Relationship to Child: _________________________________
|
Date Completed: _____________________________________
|
PRAGMATICS CHECKLIST: Check the Column That Best Fits the Child's Language Skills
Pragmatic Objective
|
Not Present
|
Uses No Words Preverbal
|
Uses One to Three Words
|
More Complex Language
|
States Needs
(I want …)
|
Makes polite requests
|
|
|
|
|
Makes choices
|
|
|
|
|
Gives description of an object wanted
|
|
|
|
|
Expresses a specific personal need
|
|
|
|
|
Requests help
|
|
|
|
|
Gives Commands
(Do as I tell you …)
|
Gives directions to play a game
|
|
|
|
|
Gives directions to make something
|
|
|
|
|
Changes the style of commands or requests depending on who the child is speaking to
and what the child wants
|
|
|
|
|
Personal
(Expresses Feelings …)
|
Identifies feelings (I'm happy)
|
|
|
|
|
Explains feelings (I'm happy because it's my birthday)
|
|
|
|
|
Provides excuses or reasons
|
|
|
|
|
Offers an opinion with support
|
|
|
|
|
Complains
|
|
|
|
|
Blames others
|
|
|
|
|
Provides pertinent information on request (two or three of the following: name, address,
phone number, birth date)
|
|
|
|
|
Interactional
(Me and You …)
|
Interacts with others in a polite manner
|
|
|
|
|
Uses appropriate social rules such as greetings, farewells, thank-yous, attention-getting
tactics
|
|
|
|
|
Attends to the speaker
|
|
|
|
|
Revises/repairs an incomplete message
|
|
|
|
|
Initiates a topic of conversation (doesn't just start talking in the middle of a topic)
|
|
|
|
|
Maintains a conversation (able to keep it going)
|
|
|
|
|
Ends a conversation (doesn't just walk away)
|
|
|
|
|
Interjects appropriately into an already established conversation with others
|
|
|
|
|
Makes apologies or gives explanations of behavior
|
|
|
|
|
Requests clarification
|
|
|
|
|
States a problem
|
|
|
|
|
Criticizes others
|
|
|
|
|
Disagrees with others
|
|
|
|
|
Compliments others
|
|
|
|
|
Makes promises
|
|
|
|
|
Wants Explanations
(Tell me Why …)
|
Asks questions to get more information
|
|
|
|
|
Asks questions to systematically gather information (20 Questions)
|
|
|
|
|
Asks questions because of curiosity
|
|
|
|
|
Asks questions to problem solve
(What should I do … ?, How do I know …?)
|
|
|
|
|
Asks questions to make predictions
(What will happen if …?)
|
|
|
|
|
Shares Knowledge and Imaginations
(I've got something to tell you …)
|
Role-plays as/with different characters
|
|
|
|
|
Role-plays with props (banana as a phone)
|
|
|
|
|
Provides a description of a situation that describes the main events
|
|
|
|
|
Correctly retells a story which has been told to them
|
|
|
|
|
Relates the content of a four- to six-frame picture story using correct events for
each frame
|
|
|
|
|
Creates an original story with a beginning, several logical events, and an end
|
|
|
|
|
Explains the relationship between two objects, actions, or situations
|
|
|
|
|
Compares and contrasts qualities of two objects, actions, or situations
|
|
|
|
|
Tells a lie
|
|
|
|
|
Expresses humor/sarcasm
|
|
|
|
|
Source: Goberis D. (Adapted from work done by Simon.[11])
The situation comprehension questions ask parents to answer yes or no to 43 different
nonverbal behaviors. Examples of nonverbal behaviors on this checklist include: looks
both ways before crossing the street, recognizes mother, looks for an object after
it disappears from sight. These questions are taken from the Situation Comprehension
subtest of the 1972 MCDI, which is a parent report questionnaire developed by Harold
Ireton. There is a newer version of the MCDI, which was published in 1994. The 1972
version of the MCDI is no longer available and no longer has an active copyright.
It is important to include these questions in this study of normal hearing children
because they were used in the large scale study of DHH children to determine nonverbal
cognitive function, which may account for variance in the pragmatics checklist.
This checklist has been distributed through paper copies; however, distributing the
Pragmatics Checklist using an online questionnaire dramatically increased the number
of responses. The survey included the exact 45 questions on the paper version with
seven additional demographic questions and 43 questions regarding situation comprehension
nonverbal skills. One significant difference between the online version and the paper
version was the fact that parents filling out the online survey must sign the parent
consent form by checking the consent box rather than giving a physical signature.
The estimated time commitment to complete the checklist was 10 minutes. All parents
of children with normal hearing who had access to the Hands & Voices Web site (http://www.handsandvoices.org/) were invited to participate in this research study. Additionally, participants were
solicited through email via the Hands & Voices email contact list. There were over
1200 contacts on the Colorado state Hands & Voices email list and the Web site is
available to 30+ state chapters as well as international chapters. It is unknown how
many of these families who are a part of the Hands & Voices organization nationally
and internationally have children with normal hearing who fall into the target age
range for this project. The parents of children with normal hearing had knowledge
about language development because they were parents who also had a child who was
DHH and if the parents resided in Colorado, they had also filled out this checklist
on their DHH child.
Results
Characteristics of the Participants
There were 109 children in the group of children with normal hearing (NH) ranging
from 2 to 7 years of age. All of the children had NH by parent report and normal cognition
based on the Situation Comprehension subscale of the MCDI. Data for the children with
NH was analyzed for groups ranging between the ages of 18 to 29 months (n = 14), 30 to 41 months (n = 19), 42 to 53 months (n = 21), 54 to 65 months (n = 23), 66 to 77 months (n = 17), 78 to 89 months (n = 11) and 90+ months (n = 4).
There were 126 participants who were DHH between the ages of 3 and 7 years. All children
had normal cognitive development and their hearing losses covered all levels. Pragmatic
language checklist data were available for (1) 93 children between 24 to 35 months
of age (most evaluations were completed at ∼33 months of age), (2) 50 children aged
36 to 47 months, (3) 102 children aged 48 to 59 months, (4) 89 children aged 60 to
71 months, (5) 82 children aged 72 to 83 months, and (6) 67 children aged 84 to 96
months.
Gender
Forty-three percent of the children with NH were male, 56% were female, and 1.8% were
unspecified. Fifty-one percent of the children who were DHH were male and 49% were
female.
Age at Testing
There was a higher percentage of 2-year-old-children who had NH (12.8%) than those
who were DHH (0.3%) and a higher percentage of 3-year-old children with NH (17.4%)
than children who were DHH (8.9%). The percentage of 4-year-old children with NH (19.3%)
and those who were DHH (20.6%) and the percentage of 5-year-old children with NH (21.2%)
and children who were DHH (23.88%) were similar. There was a higher proportion of
6-year-old children who were DHH (24.5%) than those that had NH (15.6%) and a higher
proportion of 7-year-old children (21.8%) who were DHH than NH (10.1%). Of the children
at 8-years-old, 3.7% had NH. There were no 8-year-old children who were DHH.
Maternal Level of Education
There were differences in the maternal level of education of the children with normal
hearing and the children who were DHH. Of the mothers of children who were DHH, 6.4%
had below a high school education as compared with none of the children with NH. Of
the mothers of children who were DHH, 36.5% had a high school diploma compared with
13.8% of the children with normal hearing. Of mothers of the children with NH, 5.5%
had an associate's degree as compared to10.3% of the children who were DHH. Of the
mothers of children who were NH, 35.8% had a bachelor's degree as compared with 32.5%
of mothers of children who were DHH. Of mothers who had children with NH, 30.5% had
a master's degree as compared with 11.9% of the mothers who had children who were
DHH. Of the mothers who had children with NH, 11% had a doctorate as compared with
1.6% of the mothers of children who were DHH. Less than 1% (0.9%) in the NH group
compared with 0.8% in the DHH group had unspecified maternal level of education.
Ethnicity of the Group
Sixty percent of the NH group and 68.3% of the DHH children were Caucasian, 1.8% of
NH and 19% of DHH children had a Hispanic/Latino background, 1.8% of NH and 5.6% of
the DHH were Asian American, 10.1% of the NH and 7.1% of the DHH children indicated
“other.” Twenty-six percent of the NH group did not specify and 0% of the DHH group
did not specify.
Languages Used in the Home
Of the NH group, 93.6% used English only as compared with 31.7% of the DHH group.
Of the NH, 6.4% indicated homes with bilingual spoken languages as compared with 4.8%
of the DHH group; 57.9% of the DHH group indicated that they used English and sign
language and 5.6% of the DHH group indicated that they were bilingual spoken language
and sign language homes.
Degree of Hearing Loss
In the DHH group there were 19.8% with mild hearing loss, 24.2% with moderate to moderate-severe
hearing loss, 32.9% with severe hearing loss, and 23.1% with profound hearing loss.
Test Results
Mastery of the items was defined as 75% of the children using complex language for
each item. Children with normal hearing mastered 44% (20 of 45) of the items using
complex language by 3 years of age. By 4 years of age, they mastered 95.5% (43 of
45) of the items. By 5 years of age they mastered 98% of the items. By 6 years, they
mastered 100% of the items with complex language. The final two items mastered were:
(1) provides information on request (name, date of birth, address [two or three items])
and (2) makes promises. The results of children who are DHH by age and the results
of children with normal hearing by age and by item can be found on the Web site www.mariondowns.com/research/pediatrics.
In contrast, children who are DHH mastered only 6.6% (3 of 45) of the items with complex
language by 6 years of age. They mastered 69% (31 of 45) of the items with complex
language by 7 years of age (see [Fig. 1]). The three items accomplished by 6 years were: (1) makes polite requests—uses words:
please, thank you, (2) expresses needs, and (3) role-plays with props.
Figure 1 Pragmatics Checklist items mastered by age.
Items not mastered with complex language by children who are DHH by 7 years of age
were: (1) provides information on request, (2) repairs incomplete sentences, (3) ends
conversations, (4) interjects, (5) apologies, (6) request clarification, (7) makes
promises, (8) asks questions to problem solve, (9) asks questions to make predictions,
(10) retells a story, (11) tells four- to six-frame picture story in right order,
(12) creates original story, (13) explains relationships between objects-action-situations,
and (14) compares and contrasts.
Differences by Degree of Hearing Loss
[Figure 1] depicts the proportion of items mastered by age by degree of hearing loss. Children
with mild hearing had the fastest development but were delayed compared with children
with normal hearing. From 3 to 6 years of age, children with moderate and moderate
severe hearing loss had mastered significantly fewer pragmatic skills with complex
language, but by 7 years of age, they were similar. Children with severe and profound
hearing loss had a similar rate of mastery of pragmatic skills through age 7. By 7
years of age, they had mastered ∼80% of the items.
Intervention Strategies
Parents often wonder what language skills they can work on in the home. This questionnaire
helps parents target specific language skills to model within the home providing natural
opportunities for the child to practice them. If the child is in an integrated environment,
the teacher questionnaire helps identify some of the strengths and weaknesses of the
child's language abilities and the teacher may then optimize language learning in
classroom situations where these strategies could be naturally practiced. Teachers
frequently judge language skills based upon the speech intelligibility of the child.
However, pragmatic language skills are often so subtle that the teacher does not identify
specific areas of delay or difficulty. In addition to the analysis of situations in
which language becomes difficult, it is also possible to determine whether the student
has sufficient vocabulary and grammatical capabilities to communicate his or her needs
and wants. If the student lacks specific content knowledge in vocabulary or syntax,
these skills can be taught through individualized intervention.
Giving Directions
Most early intervention specialists and speech-language pathologists (SLPs) work on
simple commands such as understanding: go get your shoes, put your shoes on, open
the door, get in the car. However, teachers and SLPs sometimes overlook having the
child give directions to others. There are some pragmatic language skills that are
important for giving directions, such as to play a game or to make something. To teach
these skills, the SLP/teacher needs to carefully analyze all of the cognitive linguistic
steps that must be understood to provide directions to another about playing a game
or making something. The child needs to understand what is in the mind of the other
individual who must learn a game or learn how to make something (i.e., theory of mind).
Can the child explain the steps well enough so that another individual can successfully
make something: a picture, a snowman, a peanut butter and jelly sandwich, or an arts
and crafts activity?
Making Something
Children also need to learn the sequence of information that they need to tell someone
else about how to make something. Even when the child understands the sequence and
is capable of making the desired food item, arts and crafts project, or object, the
child may be unable to explain to another how to do it. Children do not always understand
what basic information is important for another person to know. Remember to teach
sequence. For a peanut butter and jelly sandwich, if the child says “peanut butter”
and omits specific instructions in the sequence, the teacher/SLP can respond with
absurdities such as, “Do I put it in my hand? Where's the bread?”
The teacher/SLP needs to identify each of the steps in the sequence. It is often helpful
to have pictures that are associated with each step in the sequence. Using a chart
with pictures that depict each step in the process, children will cross out each step
as they either do it, indicating comprehension, or as they tell another to do it,
demonstrating the pragmatic expressive language skill. Then the teacher/SLP puts the
items back in the right sequence to review. The order may also be jumbled to demonstrate
that the end result will not be correct.
The teacher could teach the child the correct sequence with picture cues and written
language: (1) get a banana, chocolate, and nuts, (2) peel the banana before cutting,
(3) cut the banana, (4) dip the banana into the chocolate, (5) roll the chocolate-covered
banana in nuts, (6) freeze the banana. Teaching requires redundancy and repetition.
Make sure to let the child make mistakes. If it isn't in order, can the task be completed?
Children need to learn that making things typically requires a specific order or sequence
of events to accomplish the goal.
Learning How to Play a Game
At very young age levels, the teacher/SLP may want to teach a child to give directions
about how to play a simple game, such as Duck Duck Goose. First, the child must consider
the number of steps. Children must sit in a circle. There is a chosen child, the goose.
How is the child chosen? Does the teacher pick the child, or does the child who suggests
the game pick the chosen child? The chosen child walks around the outside of the circle.
The child taps each child and says “duck” as he or she taps. The child must choose
a child to be the goose. When a child is chosen as the goose, this child must get
up quickly and run around the circle. The child who has chosen the child to be the
goose also begins to run around the circle and whoever reaches the empty spot first
and sits down, is no longer the goose. The chosen child who is the new goose must
think about whom she or he will choose. The child must not show by eye gaze or pointing
or any other hint who the new chosen child will be. If the goose does indicate who
she or he will chose too early, then she or he will beat the chosen child to the open
spot and the chosen child becomes the new the goose. The child must keep a secret.
This is an opportunity to teach what a secret is.
The teacher/SLP may start out with a chart. The children take turns explaining how
to play the game. The teacher/SLP may scaffold the activity by asking “What's first?
What's second? What's third?” If the child simply says, “tap, tap, tap,” the teacher/SLP
must indicate that there is some missing information. “What's missing? Hmm,” the teacher
can then give some indication of the thinking process that should be used. It is important
for the teacher/parent to teach the child the thought process: “Who should I pick?
It's a secret. Don't tell anyone. Don't make eye contact, others will notice. Don't
point at the child you pick. If the child knows that you have chosen him or her, he
or she will be prepared to run around the circle and you may not get there first.”
The SLP or teacher may introduce an absurdity. “Can I pick the table? Why not? If
I walk around the circle, I need to tap a person. If I tap a chair, the chair cannot
get up and run around the circle to try to sit down in the open space first.”
This is an excellent opportunity for activities at home, such as how to teach games
like Concentration, Candy Land, or card games to a sibling or a friend. Other games
that can be described besides board games or card games are games with a ball, simple
games like Duck Duck Goose, or games with teams.
Teaching Perspective Taking
It is important for children to learn how to understand what is in the minds of others.
Teach “why” questions. Teach children how it makes them feel. Ask questions such as,
“Does it make you feel angry? Sad? Or disappointed? Why are you upset? Are your feelings
hurt? Which is better, Grab it or ask politely? If I take it from you, how does it
feel?”
Teach about Choices and What the Consequences Are for Each Choice
With children who have high language skills, it is important to teach verbal mediation
skills and the language appropriate for these pragmatic skills. For children with
lower language skills, the teacher/SLP may want to use pantomime and pictures.
Playing 20 Questions
Although children with NH learn how to play the game 20 questions without having to
learn the steps, children who are DHH often need specific instructions. How do you
select the question to be asked? Which type of question is better, “Is it a dog? Is
it alive? Is it an animal?” When the question is answered, what should the child do
with the answer? Does the child eliminate any possibilities? It is easiest to learn
how to play the game if the options are limited? With pictures of the possible answers,
after the question is answered, it is possible to eliminate some of the pictures.
Can the child explain why these answers would be eliminated? The child should cross
out the pictures of answers that cannot be the chosen one because of the answer given
to the previous question. Then the child needs to look at the remaining pictures and
formulate a question that will either provide the information about which one is the
chosen answer or eliminate more of the potential answers. The SLP may need to teach
the child what questions might yield the best ability to eliminate answers. These
questions are frequently category questions, such as “Is it alive?” or “Is it an animal?”
The child must learn how to keep information in his or her head to use for formulating
the next question. The child must understand how to categorize things. Categorization
often begins with visual obvious characteristics, such as the color, “Is it white?”
Or the size, “Is it big?” Or, “Is it alive?” Or, “How do animals move? Do they fly?
Swim? Walk?”
Recognizing a Falsehood
To survive in this society, it is important that children understand the difference
between truth and a lie. Children need to learn when they are being “tricked.” They
need to understand what it means to be gullible, so that social victimization can
be prevented. Role-play can consist of pretending to be a trickster, such as “coyote”
in the southwest of America. Should I believe the trickster? Are there any clues provided
about tricksters? What strategies can be used to determine whether something is true
or something is a lie? How does the child know when to believe someone? A child cannot
understand truth without understanding a lie.
Persuasion
Why is it important for children to learn how to be persuasive? What are some real
life examples when the child may find it important to persuade someone? A child may
want to know how to persuade his or her parents to allow them to do something or get
something; permission to do a particular activity, go to a party, go to get ice cream,
etc. A child may also want to persuade other children to let him or her play with
them. What are some strategies that can be used? How can the child assess whether
or not the strategy is successful?
Telling a Story in Sequence
Children who are DHH often need to learn how to relate stories of events so that someone
else can understand the story. Notebooks that go back and forth between the SLP/teacher
and the home can include information about what events may be of importance to the
child that occurred at home or after school. The SLP/teacher can then ask the child
to tell them information about what happened. Then it is possible to determine whether
the child is capable of telling a story or event in a logical order with sufficient
information for others to understand. A parent will also know if something significant
or important occurred that day—so that if the child begins to talk about an event,
the parent will be knowledgeable enough to help support the child in learning how
to relate information to another person. Because the teacher/SLP knows the event,
she or he is also able to determine what information has been omitted or is incomplete.
The teacher/SLP will want to determine if there is understanding of cause and effect.
Information about comparing and contrasting may also be important when relating the
information. Storytelling should improve if both the teacher/SLP and parent(s) provide
appropriate modeling with emphasis on the components omitted by the child.
Defense in the Face of False Accusation
Is the child able to use language that provides defense in the face of a false accusation?
Does the child understand the situation sufficiently to identify what defense would
prove his or her innocence? Statements (e.g., “He took the toy. I didn't take the
toy. I didn't want the toy. I wasn't in the vicinity. I don't have the toy. I didn't
want the toy.”) that provide evidence that the accusation is false could include a
variety of perspectives.
Alternate Points of View
The child does not have to have the same point of view as another (e.g., “I like this
book or song”). The child does not have to like the book or song (e.g., “He hates
this book”). Questions that could stimulate thinking about alternative points of view
could assist the child in thinking about another's perspective: “Why did you like
this book? What was your favorite part? What is another student's favorite part? Does
everyone like the book?”
Revision of Unclear Message
First the child must be able to identify when the message was unclear to another person.
If they can identify miscommunication, then they can supply options. The teacher/SLP
should model for the child. If the child says, “boat,” the SLP/teacher could question,
“Do I want the boat? Do I want to eat the boat? Do you want to get on the boat? Is
it a blue boat?”
Ability to Answer Questions
A response needs to be appropriate, not just a response. Imitation is not a response
to a question. The SLP/teacher needs to model both correct and incorrect, or model
an absurd response.
Maintaining a Topic
Sample questions might include “Tell me three things about what you did last night.
What kind of things did you see? Hear? Can you picture that in your head? Can you
picture what would happen in the restaurant? Can you picture what would happen in
a store?”
Theory of Mind
Children often need to be explicitly taught how to think about predicting what is
in the mind of another person. Children who are DHH often think that what they know,
others also know. They need to be taught that a person knows what they have experienced
and if they have not had access to specific information, they will make mistakes.