Keywords:
Aphasia - Language Tests - Neuropsychological Tests
Palavras-chave:
Afasia - Testes de Linguagem - Testes Neuropsicológicos
INTRODUCTION
The ability to retrieve names is one of the most sensitive measurements for assessing
language[1] and is one that tends to persist during follow-ups on language disorders[1]. These impairments contribute to the differential diagnosis of the subtypes of aphasia,
together with oral comprehension, oral production (speech fluency) and repetition
failures.
Naming a pictured or a real object is a complex process and involves a number of relatively
distinct cognitive processes and mental representations[2],[3]. Firstly, it requires recognition of the visual stimulus as an instance of a familiar
concept. The meaning of the stimulus then has to be accessed in a long-term semantic
memory representation. Activation of its lexical representation, among possible competing
alternatives, is also required. Retrieval of the exact phonological word form takes
place, followed by activation of the motor program and effective articulation, thus
leading to word production[4]. There is evidence that these processes seem to be somehow interactive[5].
The representations and processes underlying naming may thus be hampered at different
levels in patients with brain lesions, thereby leading to distinct patterns of performance.
Patients may have difficulties at the visual recognition level, with an agnosic type
of behavior, such that they are unable to match drawings of objects, or to recognize
partial drawings or objects presented from an unusual perspective. Some other patients
may access the structural visual description but cannot access the lexical-semantic
stores and might perform poorly in semantic tasks or might produce semantic paraphasia.
Still others cannot retrieve the phonological word form, thus presenting the tip-of-the-tongue
phenomenon, with pauses, phonological errors or attempts to name by using successive
conduites d’ approche. Therefore, both the success of naming and the type of errors produced can be informative
about the impaired cognitive processes and give cues for language rehabilitation.
As a rule, naming tests for aphasia or cognitive assessment consist of presentation
of real objects (Lisbon Aphasia Assessment Battery (BAAL)[6] or line drawings of objects, actions or other stimuli (body parts, food, etc.)[7],[8],[9],[10]. However, these types of stimuli may not be suitable for certain clinical contexts
such as bedside assessment or emergency situations (before endovascular treatments
or thrombolysis) in which clinicians need to have quick access to images that can
be presented. Nowadays, this is easily achieved through accessing images on a smartphone
or tablet. The same occurs during distant assessment of aphasia, particularly when
there is risk of infection, such as during the COVID-19 pandemic. Therefore it is
relevant to know which types of stimuli are more appropriate and to what extent they
impact on the subject’s performance.
It is well recognized that the ability to retrieve a name is related to several variables,
comprising word frequency, stimulus familiarity, degree of abstraction or imageability,
age at acquisition and recency[11]. Moreover, the color and the number of visual dimensions (two or three) are involved.
Studies on healthy individuals have shown that 2D, 3D and colored images may be treated
differently in the brain, particularly among subjects with low levels of literacy[12],[13],[14]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no comparative studies have been conducted
among people with aphasia, between objects in two and three dimensions.
The aim of this study was to investigate whether the ability to name objects among
individuals with aphasia is influenced by the dimensions of the visual stimuli, through
comparing real objects with digitally presented color photographs. Additionally, it
was sought to understand which clinical variables are involved in success in naming,
such as the order of presentation of the stimuli, number of years of formal education
and length of time post-onset.
METHODS
Study design
This was a prospective cross-sectional observational study comparing the accuracy
of naming performance between individuals with aphasia and controls, with regard to
two types of colored visual stimuli.
Population
Clinical sample
The subjects were consecutive patients with aphasia due to stroke who were admitted
to a stroke unit or who were undergoing speech therapy, in a university hospital.
Patient inclusion and exclusion criteria
The patients included presented the following characteristics: diagnosis of aphasia,
either in the acute or in the chronic stage; age above 18 years; ability to cooperate
in the evaluation; and ability to provide informed consent in person or through a
caregiver. The following exclusion criteria were used: poor cooperation; visual impairment;
absence of speech or speech reduced to a stereotype; or a previous (before stroke)
or current diagnosis of dementia. The diagnosis of aphasia was clinical among the
acute patients and was made by acute-care neurologists during bedside examination
and through use of the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale[15]. For chronic patients attending the speech therapy department, their diagnosis was
supported by speech therapists and was quantified though the BAAL[6], which is the national gold-standard instrument for aphasia assessment in Portugal.
Controls
Individuals above 18 years of age, with no history of neurological or psychiatric
illness and with a Mini-Mental State Examination[16] score within the normal values for age and education, were included as a control
group.
All participants or, in the case of patients with aphasia, their caregivers, gave
written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study
was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Academic Center of Medicine of Lisbon.
Procedures
All participants, with aphasia and controls, were assessed by the same evaluator (JF),
through two oral naming tasks of 24 items each. These tasks consisted of naming a
set of common real objects and a set of digital color photographs of the same objects
that were presented in standard views on the screen of a tablet. The stimuli were
selected from a wider set by an evaluation panel (of speech therapists, psychologists
and neurologists), taking into account the quality of the photograph, difficulty/familiarity
of the stimulus and word frequency. The word frequency of the items selected was determined
in accordance with a written frequency scale for the European Portuguese Language,
consisting of 12 levels[17]. The items selected belong to levels 4 (16.7%), 5 (41.7%), 6 (25%) and 7 (16.7%)
of this scale, which represent medium-to-high frequency of the use of nouns in European
Portuguese. Level 1 on this scale consists of the lowest-frequency words and level
12, the highest-frequency words (grammatical items).
The objects and photographs were displayed one by one, always following the same order.
The sequence of presentation, i.e. real objects followed by photographs or photographs
followed by real objects, was randomized to control for learning. An interference
task was applied between the two sets, consisting of a spontaneous speech production
test. The participants’ responses were audiotaped for analysis. No aids were provided,
and no touching of objects was allowed. The participants’ first response was quoted,
and the evaluator did not provide any feedback during application of the test.
Statistical analysis
We used descriptive statistics to characterize the continuous variables of age, education
level and test scores (using the mean or median, with standard deviation or interquartile
range) and to describe the categorical variables of gender and case type (acute or
chronic) (using percentages). The naming scores from the real objects and the photographs
were compared by means of Student’s t test. ANOVA for repeated measurements was used
to investigate the difference between the two tasks, controlled for presentation order.
Multiple linear regression, using the enter method, was carried out to evaluate the
effects of age, education level and length of time post-stroke onset, on each naming
score. Results were considered significant when p < 0.05. The statistical analysis was performed using the Statistical Package for
the Social Sciences software (version 24.0)[18].
RESULTS
A total of 40 subjects with aphasia (12 men), with an average age of 62.4 years and
8.5 years of education were included ([Table 1]). The majority had suffered ischemic stroke (N = 33). Twenty-four patients were
observed within the first eight days and 16 were observed in the chronic period after
stroke. Apart from the length of time post-stroke onset, there were no significant
differences between the acute and chronic patients in terms of age, gender, education
level or type of stroke, or in their performance in each of the naming tasks ([Table 2]).
Table 1
Demographic data and total scores obtained among patients and controls.
|
Control subjects (N = 50)
|
Aphasia patients (N = 40)
|
Test
|
p
|
95% CI
|
Age (years) mean ± SD (range)
|
67.3 ± 15.7 (24-89)
|
62.4 ± 17.3 (24-91)
|
-1.405
|
p = ns
|
-11,962; 2,062
|
Education level (years) mean ± SD
|
8.1 ± 4.8
|
8.5 ± 5.3
|
0.372
|
p = ns
|
-1.738; 2.538
|
Education level (years) 0-6 (%) > 6 (%)
|
25 (50.0%) 25 (50.0%)
|
21 (52.5%) 19 (47.5%)
|
X2 = 0.056
|
p = ns
|
0.481; 2.540
|
Gender M (%) F (%)
|
14 (28%) 36 (72%)
|
12 (30%) 28 (70%)
|
X2 = 0.043
|
p = ns
|
0.441; 2.753
|
Objects score mean ± SD (max = 24)
|
24.0 ± 0.0
|
15.8 ± 7.6
|
-6.813
|
p = 0.000
|
-10.602; -5.748
|
Photographs score mean ± SD (max = 24)
|
23.9 ± 0.4
|
14.0 ± 7.3
|
-8.596
|
p = 0.000
|
-12.291; -7.609
|
CI: confidence interval; ns: not significant; M: Male; F: Female.
Table 2
Demographic data, clinical data and total scores: comparison between the length of
time post-onset.
|
Acute (≤ 8 days)
|
Chronic (> 8 days)
|
Statistic
|
N
|
24
|
16
|
|
Age (years) mean ± SD
|
65.8 ± 16.0
|
57.2 ± 18.4
|
p = ns
|
Education level (years) mean ± SD
|
7.4 ± 5.2
|
10.2 ± 5.0
|
p = ns
|
Education level (years) 0-6 (%) > 6 (%)
|
15 (62.5%) 9 (37.5%)
|
6 (37.5%) 10 (62.5%)
|
p = ns
|
Gender M (%) F (%)
|
8 (33%) 16 (66%)
|
4(25%) 12 (75%)
|
p = ns
|
Length of time post-onset (days) mean ± SD
|
3.9 ± 2.2
|
517.4 (717.1)
|
t = 3.531 (38); p = 0.001
|
Stroke Ischemic (%) Hemorrhagic (%)
|
21:2
|
12:3
|
p = ns
|
Objects score mean ± SD (max = 24)
|
16.1 ± 7.9
|
15.4 ± 7.3
|
p = ns
|
Photographs score mean ± SD (max =24)
|
13.7 ± 7.7
|
14.3 ± 6.9
|
p = ns
|
Stimulus total
|
29.8 ± 15.2
|
29.8 ± 14.1
|
p = ns
|
ns: not significant; M: Male; F: Female.
The control group consisted of 50 subjects who were matched for age, gender and education
with the participants with aphasia, as depicted in [Table 1]. The controls performed at ceiling levels and outperformed the participants with
aphasia in both naming tests ([Table 1]).
The participants with aphasia obtained a significantly higher score through naming
the objects than through naming the photographs (t = 3.720 (39); p = 0.001) ([Table 3]). No difference was found among the controls. A more detailed analysis on the patients’
performance revealed that only two out of the total of 24 items presented a significant
difference between the two types of stimulus. These consisted of a mirror (F = 11.323;
p = 0.002) and glasses (F = 4.944; p = 0.032) ([Table 4]). Nonetheless, the overall score difference remained after eliminating these two
items. Presentation order (objects-photographs or photographs-objects) was not associated
with differences in naming scores ([Table 5]).
Table 3
Comparison of performance achieved by the controls and the patients with acute or
chronic aphasia in the two naming tasks (repeated-measurement ANOVA).
Group
|
N
|
Objects Mean ± SD; median
|
Photos Mean ± SD; median
|
F
|
P
|
Controls
|
50
|
24.0 ± 0.0;24
|
23.9 ± 0.4;24
|
3.769
|
0.06
|
Patients with aphasia
|
40
|
15.83 ± 7.59; 18.50
|
13.95 ± 7.31; 15.00
|
13.836
|
0.001
|
Acute aphasia
|
24
|
16.1 ± 7.9
|
13.7 ± 7.7
|
9.803
|
0.005
|
Chronic aphasia
|
16
|
15.8 ± 7.6
|
14.0 ± 7.3
|
4.765
|
0.045
|
Table 4
Comparison between objects and photos by means of ANOVA for repeated measurements.
Stimulus
|
Objects Mean ± SD; median
|
Photos Mean ± SD; median
|
F
|
P
|
Pencil
|
0.78 ± 0.42; 1.0
|
0.68 ± 0.47; 1.0
|
2.053
|
0.160
|
Pin
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.40 ± 0.50; 0.0
|
3.468
|
0.70
|
Match
|
0.73 ± 0.45; 1.0
|
0.60 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
1.970
|
0.168
|
Hairbrush
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.50 ± 0.50; 0.5
|
0.814
|
0.372
|
Coin
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.68 ± 0.47; 1.0
|
0.109
|
0.743
|
Swiss switchblade/penknife
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.000
|
1.000
|
Fork
|
0.68 ± 0.47; 1.0
|
0.60 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
1.000
|
0.323
|
Scissors
|
0.63 ± 0.49; 1.0
|
0.65 ± 0.48; 1.0
|
0.109
|
0.743
|
Mirror
|
0.75 ± 0.44; 1.0
|
0.45 ± 0.50; 0.0
|
11.323
|
0.002*
|
Perfume bottle
|
0.45 ± 0.50; 0.0
|
0.45 ± 0.50; 0.0
|
0.000
|
1.000
|
Bill/banknote
|
0.63 ± 0.49; 1.0
|
0.53 ± 0.51; 1.0
|
2.053
|
0.160
|
Stamp
|
0.65 ± 0.48; 1.0
|
0.53 ± 0.51; 1.0
|
2.910
|
0.096
|
Spoon
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.60 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
2.053
|
0.160
|
Matchbox
|
0.60 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.089
|
0.767
|
Doorbell
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
2.910
|
0.096
|
Pen
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.55 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
3.162
|
0.083
|
Key
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.58 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
1.970
|
0.168
|
Hair comb
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.000
|
1.000
|
Clock
|
0.68 ± 0.47; 1.0
|
0.60 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
1.295
|
0.262
|
Glasses
|
0.78 ± 0.42; 1.0
|
0.63 ± 0.49; 1.0
|
4.944
|
0.032*
|
Glass cup
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.73 ± 0.45; 1.0
|
0.109
|
0.743
|
Clothespin/clothes peg
|
0.55 ± 0.50; 1.0
|
0.45 ± 0.50; 0.0
|
2.053
|
0.160
|
Teacup
|
0.65 ± 0.48; 1.0
|
0.70 ± 0.46; 1.0
|
0.281
|
0.599
|
Lightbulb
|
0.68 ± 0.47; 1.0
|
0.65 ± 0.48; 1.0
|
0.089
|
0.767
|
Total without mirror/glasses
|
14.30 ± 7.02; 16.50
|
12.88 ± 6.66; 14.0
|
9.215
|
0.004
|
Table 5
Demographic data, clinical data and total scores: comparison between education levels.
|
Education level ≤ 6 years
|
Education level > 6 years
|
Statistic
|
N
|
21
|
19
|
|
Age (years) mean ± SD
|
66.5 ± 17.9
|
57.7 ± 15.8
|
p = ns
|
Education level (years) mean ± SD
|
4.1 ± 1.2
|
13.4 ± 3.1
|
t = -12.983 (38) p = 0.000
|
Gender M (%) F (%)
|
9 (43%) 12 (57%)
|
3 (16%) 16 (84%)
|
p = ns
|
Length of time post-onset (days) mean ± SD
|
35.6 ± 88.7
|
401.3 ± 697.4
|
t = -2.385 (38) p = 0.022
|
Stroke Ischemic (%) Hemorrhagic (%)
|
20 (95%) 1 (5%)
|
13 (68%) 4 (21%)
|
p = ns
|
Objects score mean ± SD (max = 24)
|
14.6 ± 7.6
|
17.2 ± 7.5
|
p = ns
|
Photographs score mean ± SD (max = 24)
|
12.4 ± 6.9
|
15.7 ± 7.6
|
p = ns
|
ns: not significant; M: Male, F: Female.
Multiple linear regression was applied to create a model for predicting naming abilities
among individuals with aphasia, based on the independent variables of age, number
of years of formal education, length of time post-onset and stimulus order. However,
the model had low predictive value and none of the independent variables was significantly
associated with performance regarding naming of objects (F(4) = 0.333: p = ns; R2 = 0.037) or colored photographs (F(4) = 0.586: p = ns; R2 = 0.063).
DISCUSSION
In this study, we found that the performance of people with aphasia was superior in
naming real objects than in naming color photographs of the same objects presented
on a screen, regardless of the length of time post-onset and education level of the
subjects. This demonstrates the importance of object dimension, and of presentation
type (real versus virtual), in aphasia testing.
Damásio et al. (1979)[19], through comparing the naming of real objects and black and white drawings, and
Reis et al. (2006)[14], through comparing color and black and white photographs, demonstrated that color
can influence the visual perception of the stimuli. Furthermore, specific difficulty
in naming 2D stimuli was described among healthy subjects with low education levels[13]. Reis et al.[6] argued that formal education was important in the cognitive process involved in
processing two-dimensional but not three-dimensional representations of common objects,
thus indicating that education influences the visual system or the interaction between
the visual and the language systems.
However, in the present study, and in the particular context of aphasia, education
level was not a predictor of performance. This means that in individuals with aphasia
there may be other factors involved, i.e. the severity of aphasia or the extent of
brain lesions. This may be particularly relevant for objects that are difficult to
represent in two dimensions, such as a mirror or glasses, which might be more difficult
to recognize in a picture. Nevertheless, the present results could not be explained
solely on the basis of those two items, given that the final score difference remained
when they were removed. Moreover, the healthy controls performed at the ceiling level
in the photograph task.
Aphasia due to stroke corresponds, in the large majority of cases, to lesions in the
region of the middle cerebral artery. This tends to spare the brain areas responsible
for the earliest levels of visual processing, in the occipital lobe, which depends
on the posterior cerebral artery. The current anatomical model of language organization
postulates that there are two main language processing pathways: a dorsal stream and
a ventral stream. These roughly support speech programming and production and speech
comprehension, respectively[20]. Fridriksson et al.[21] analyzed the symptoms of aphasia according to this model and found that the naming
abilities of people with aphasia could be predicted by the degree of damage to an
extensive cortical network and did not correspond to a specific localization. However,
the ventral language system gently overlaps with the visual recognition pathways and
the semantic areas of the left middle and inferior temporal lobes. Hence, this system
may have an impact on fine object recognition. In addition, more anterior frontal
lesions may interfere with abstraction abilities, which are also required in order
to infer a meaning from less ecological representation. In the future, it may be of
interest to understand whether difficulty in naming 2D items is more likely to be
observed in ventral than in dorsal language pathway lesions.
It is true that in many clinical situations, such as in the emergency room or at the
bedside of a patient, it is easier to present different types of stimuli on a tablet
or smartphone. However, only 2D stimuli that were previously tested or validated among
people with aphasia should be used. The same applies when subjects with aphasia are
evaluated at a distance, such as is taking place during periods of confinements or
when motor impairments limit the transportation of patients to the assessment location.
We acknowledge that there were some limitations to our study, namely the small sample
studied and the limited number of stimuli. Thus, we stress that there is a need to
confirm these results in a larger series and in populations from different cultures.
We did not include neuroimaging data and the only detailed neurolinguistic analysis
(severity of aphasia and degree of syntactic impairment, for instance) came from the
sample of chronic patients, which was insufficient to analyze.
Even though these results need to be confirmed in larger samples of patients from
other contexts, we believe they can be useful in relation to evaluating patients with
aphasia at the bedside or at a distance, with a view to taking into account the possibility
that patients’ naming scores from images are underestimated in comparison with testing
using real objects.
In future research, it will be of interest to ascertain whether individuals’ performance
in naming real objects and color photographs is influenced by the lesion location
and the type of aphasia.